• Users Online: 494
  • Home
  • Print this page
  • Email this page
Home About us Editorial board Ahead of print Current issue Search Archives Submit article Instructions Subscribe Contacts Login 


 
 Table of Contents  
REVIEWS
Year : 2019  |  Volume : 5  |  Issue : 3  |  Page : 136-141

Application of Bayesian Analysis in Medical Diagnosis


1 Department of Neurology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, New Delhi, India
2 National Drug Dependence Treatment Centre, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, New Delhi, India
3 Department of Cardiology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, New Delhi, India

Date of Submission16-Aug-2019
Date of Decision26-Sep-2019
Date of Acceptance26-Nov-2019
Date of Web Publication20-Dec-2019

Correspondence Address:
Dr. Rajiv Narang
Department of Cardiology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, Ansari Nagar, New Delhi - 110 029
India
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/jpcs.jpcs_51_19

Rights and Permissions
  Abstract 


In this work, we outlined the application of the Bayesian technique for integrating the results of multiple tests while treating any disease. We provided an overview of the fundamental concept of Bayesian analysis in making an inference about a phenomenon using a concordance between available information and prior knowledge. An attempt is being made to demonstrate the applicability using core aspects, viz., nature of probability, parameters, and inferential procedure used to draw inference about the population characteristic under both paradigms. Here, we tried to sketch the underlying steps and assumptions regarding use of the Bayes theorem and the analytical techniques that can be used to analyze and interpret medical data while treating any disease.

Keywords: Bayes factor, Chi-square tests, confidence interval, credible interval, posterior distribution, P value


How to cite this article:
Verma V, Mishra AK, Narang R. Application of Bayesian Analysis in Medical Diagnosis. J Pract Cardiovasc Sci 2019;5:136-41

How to cite this URL:
Verma V, Mishra AK, Narang R. Application of Bayesian Analysis in Medical Diagnosis. J Pract Cardiovasc Sci [serial online] 2019 [cited 2020 Feb 28];5:136-41. Available from: http://www.j-pcs.org/text.asp?2019/5/3/136/273743




  Introduction Top


In medical treatments, clinicians and nurses very often have to make various complex and critical decisions during the diagnosis of the patients. In reality, these decisions are full of uncertainty and unpredictability. However, based on the available information, obtained from various clinical and diagnostic tests and situation of the patient, both clinicians and nurses try to reduce their uncertainty in clinical decisions and attempts to shift to the predictability of the chance of improvement in patient's condition. Here, best estimates are obtained using the available information on clinical and diagnostic tests, and the situation of the patient acted as light of evidence while making a decision. For the clinicians, it is of more concerned that they are capable enough in understanding and interpreting the importance of prediction probability of particular outcomes based on the recommended clinical and diagnostic tests. The scenario can be visualized through Example 1, where a researcher is interested in determining the association of smoking as a risk factor for the development of myocardial infarction [Table 1].
Table 1: Cross-tabulation of smoking habit with occurrence of myocardial infarction

Click here to view


Example 1

Myocardial infarction is one of the most leading causes of death worldwide from coronary artery disease (CAD). Existing literature has shown that risk for myocardial infarction is higher among smokers compared to nonsmokers. To determine the association of smoking as risk factor for development of myocardial infarction, a medical researcher conducted a case–control study on 100 patients, where 50 patients with myocardial infarction within 1 year on onset and 50 controls were recruited from a tertiary care hospital [Table 1]. Suppose p1 is the proportion of patients in control group affected from myocardial infarction and p2 as the same for group exposed. He hypothesized his perception under null as the occurrence of myocardial infarction is independent of smoking habit against one-sided alternative as myocardial infarction is higher among smokers than nonsmokers.

[ININE:1]

In the control group, 0 myocardial infarction cases are found; in the exposed group, there are three. As P = 0.1212, based on one-sided Fisher's exact test, is greater than 0.05 significance level, we do not reject the null hypothesis that the smoking habit is independent of myocardial infarction.

Clinical decisions are based on the probability theory because it acts as a key element required to establish a process of reasoning.[1] The challenges that occurred to clinicians and nurses are in estimating the probability based on the process and its related quantification by relating the proposition obtained from the available information with the values of probabilities.[2]

In medical diagnosis, the probability defined as the “degree of belief” or plausibility that supports the given proposition as the decision. The decisions are made using the prior experiences and support of available information in terms of various diagnostic and medical records. The situation can be understood practically, using Example 2 as an extension of Example 1.

Example 2 (continuation of Example 1)

A cardiologist may claim that three myocardial infarction cases in 50 patients are definitely in cardiological aspects being significant. This belief may be based on the information from another study in the same department in the previous year in which myocardial infarction has never shown up in control patients. If there were 400 historical controls in addition to the 50 concurrent controls, none with myocardial infarction, then the contingency table takes the following form [Table 2].
Table 2: Cross-tabulation of smoking habit with occurrence of myocardial infarction with additional controls

Click here to view


Now, the one-sided P value = 0.0009, based on one-sided Fisher's exact test, is less than 0.05 significance level, we reject the null hypothesis that the smoking habit is independent of the myocardial infarction, which makes both the statistical and cardiological importance more compatible.

In one context, Example 2 as compared to Example 1, we have shown that the decision based on frequentist principle may change from acceptance to rejection, whereas the other aspect which suggests incorporation of existing information along with the obtained information will help to achieve a more realistic conclusion. In the frequentist paradigm, it is generally inappropriate[3],[4] to gather historical and concurrent information and to quantify one's prior experiences and learning's in light of available information.

In frequentist statistics, the probability of occurrence of an event is defined in the long run of the experiment (i.e., the experiment is repeated under the same conditions to obtain the outcome).

Example 3

Let us consider that a coin is tossed and our interest lies in estimation of the fairness of the coin, as presented in [Table 3]. To check the unbiased behavior of the coin, the experiment is theoretically repeated infinite number of times but practically done with a stopping intention.
Table 3: The frequency distribution of heads tossed from a fair coin

Click here to view


As we know that probability of getting a head on tossing a fair coin is 0.5, then the number of heads will represent the actual number of heads obtained. Here, difference denotes the difference between 0.5 times (number of tosses) − number of heads obtained [Table 3].

An important thing is to note that, though the difference between the actual number of heads and expected number of heads (50% of number of tosses) increases as the number of tosses are increased, the proportion of number of heads to total number of tosses approaches to 0.5 (for a fair coin). However, based on the frequentist principle, it is not possible to estimate or update the unbiasedness of the coin using the limited tosses, and theoretically repeating the experiment infinite number of times is practically impossible.

To incorporate the prior experience with the available information, statisticians tried for alternative procedures that will update their knowledge based on the available evidence so that decisions became more effective and certain. The Bayesian method was an outcome of such an effort. This procedure was introduced by Thomas Bayes and Laplace in the 18th century and developed by statisticians and philosophers in the 20th century. Bayesian approaches were considered as the mathematical manipulation of uncertainty as it provides a way of reasoning coherently in the world around us, in the face of uncertainty,[5] where probabilities are quantified by the plausibility or truth of a suggestion that is conditional on the available or gathered information.[1]

The probability interpretation of an event can be understood in two ways: first, through direct ways (frequentist), where we know the previous circumstances and required the probability of an event, and second, through unconventional ways by using the inverse method (Bayesian), where we know the event which has happened and requires the probability which is resulted from any particular set of circumstances under which it might have happened.


  Example 4 Top


The probability of a certain medical test being positive is 90% if a patient has disease. 1% of the population has the disease, and the test records a false positive 5% of the time. If we receive a positive test, what is our probability of having disease?

According to the frequentist way of defining, the probability of having disease is 1%, as either the person has the disease or not. On the other hand, the Bayesian suggests that there is a prior probability of 1% that the person has the disease. This probability should be updated in light of the new data as



we can obtain the probability that the person, whose test was positive and also has the disease, i.e., P(Disease | Test+) as





=0.15.

The result suggests that probability that the person has the disease given that the test was found to be positive is 15%.

Bayes theorem has used widely to draw inferences in various clinical trials[3],[6] and healthcare evaluation.[7] Among various applications of Bayes' theorem has also used for outcomes' analysis and assessments such as cardiac arrest,[8] stress testing,[9] estimation of cardiac biventricular volumes,[10] perioperative cardiac risk assessment,[11] percutaneous coronary intervention strategies in cardiogenic shock,[12] and evaluation of the noninferiority of transcatheter aortic valve replacement as compared with surgical valve replacement[13] and its application in stress electrocardiography, thallium scintigraphy, technetium blood-pool scintigraphy, and cardiac fluoroscopy.[14]

The present work outlined an overview of methodological aspects of the Bayesian technique and compared the classical or frequentist approach. Section 2 presented a summary of introductory background about the definition of probability based on the Bayes theorem. The differences in the Bayesian and frequentist approaches are presented in Section 3. This section discussed how Bayesian and frequentist methods define probability, estimation procedures, and performance evaluation.


  Fundamentals Of Bayesian Methodology Top


There is no fundamental distinction between unobserved events and unknown parameter of interest in the Bayesian paradigm and are defined using the available quantities and information required to describe probability of occurrence of that event. In, the existing information available before observing data is called prior distribution, and after observing the data, the update in the information due to additional information of data is called posterior distribution. The distributional assumptions that lead to obtaining inferences in Bayesian procedure are as follows:[15],[16]

  1. A specific parametric form is essential to describe the distribution of the observations given the parameter value, which is commonly known as a likelihood function
  2. Since the parameter is assumed as an unknown quantity rather than being fixed as of classical inference, the prior distribution on the parameter of interest unconditional on the data must be given.


The essential steps that usually followed to draw inferences about the characteristic of the unknown parameter under the Bayesian thinking are:

  1. Specify a probability model, which contains some prior information about the unknown parameter
  2. Update information about the unknown parameter through conditioning their probability model given the observed data. Here, the data values are assumed to be exchangeable, i.e., the model will not be changed by reordering the data value. This exchangeability assumption assures that the data generation process is conditional on the unknown model parameter and will be the same for every generated data
  3. Evaluate the performance of the model to the available or observed data and sensitivity of the conclusion to the assumption about the model.


Methodologically, Bayesian modeling and Bayesian data analysis are approaches of incorporation of prior information and utilization of observed data to draw inference about the parameters. Here, data are stored as a likelihood function, which describes the strength of support obtained from the observations for the various possible values of the parameter. In the Bayesian paradigm, the prior information about the unknown parameter of the statistical model plays a very crucial role, which needs to be identified and expressed in the form of a prior distribution. Generally, prior distributions are classified as either informative prior, where prior information is genuine and provides the best explanations of its strength and relation to the observed data, or noninformative prior that are also known as improper or weak prior, where the credibility of prior information is lacking and is defined based on the belief before obtaining the new data. This information obtained from the prior and the data are then synthesized to produce posterior distribution, which expresses the enhancement in the knowledge about the parameter after obtaining the data. Bayesian methodology can be mathematically formulated as, if D denotes the data point and θ some model parameter,



Where P(θ) is the prior probability of θ before observing any information about D, P (D|θ) is the likelihood function which denotes the probability of observing D conditioned on θ, and P|D) is the posterior probability of θ after observing D.


  Methodological Differences in Paradoxes of Frequentist and Bayesian Paradigm Top


One of the main objectives of medical research is to infer a population phenomenon based on the information available in terms of some data from it. For exploring more information about the behavior and pattern of changes in the population characteristic(s) using the available or obtained sample, in addition to classical summaries, a specific branch of statistics deals with that is called the inferential statistics. In inferential statistical, we expand our horizon beyond the classical statistics with various techniques of estimation of parameters, comparison of their closeness, and changes with population value. There are two different philosophical aspects of inferential statistics, viz., classical or frequentist inference and Bayesian inference.

For clarity under the Bayesian approach, we first need to understand the basic differences between Bayesian and frequentist inference. This section addressed the nature of probability, parameters, and inferences under the two approaches. In the Bayesian paradigm parameter of interest, θ, is assumed as an unknown quantity rather than being fixed as of frequentist methodology. The core difference between Bayesian and frequentist methodologies in contrast to the estimation of the parameter of interest says θ. In the present section, we highlighted some of the important aspects, viz., nature of probability, parameters, and inferential procedure used to draw inference about the population characteristic under both Bayesian and frequentist based on the obtained samples. The core differences are as follows:

Interpretation of probability

Frequentist defined probability as the number of times an event observed (D) when an infinite series of the trial has performed under identical situation and condition and usually symbolized as P(D|θ). However, in Bayesian paradigm,[2] probability is defined as degree of belief of the observer before or after the data observed.

The definition of probability as stated in Bayes theorem of equation (1) is subjective and not the same as objectively (frequentist) defined as “the relative frequency of an occurrence of the event from a large number of repeated trials.” The frequentist definition raised some interesting epistemological issues regarding probability theory, such as how one can define the probability density function of a parameter on the basis a single data set. These questions can easily address using Bayesian interpretation of probability.

The basic difference between frequentist and Bayesian methodology was that frequentist defines probability as the frequency of repeated occurrence of an event (perhaps hypothetical), whereas for Bayesians, probability is defined as the degree of certainty of the occurrence of an event. Broadly speaking, for frequentist model, parameters are fixed quantities and data are random; on the other hand, for Bayesians model, parameter(s) is a random quantity and data are considered as fixed.

Example 5

To detect the CAD (D), usually cardiac fluoroscopy (T) test is opted, where the number of major coronary arteries (left anterior descending, circumflex, and right coronary artery) was reviewed in the routine heart's fluoroscopy to trace the deposition of radiodense[14] in the heart. The available information, which was obtained in terms of sensitivity, (P[T+|D+]), specificity, (P[T−|D−]), and prior information (pretest likelihood) such as chance of occurrence of CAD, P(D+), under Bayesian paradigm, was utilized to calculate the posttest likelihood for the occurrence of CAD as.



The posttest likelihood or posterior probabilities provide more information than that obtained in pretest likelihood and available information.

Fixed and variable quantities

In frequentist methodology, the obtained observations have considered as an independent and identically distributed random sample from a population, where the population parameters are unknown but fixed by nature. On the other hand, in Bayesian analysis, the observed data are considered as fixed quantity or realization of the population characteristic. Here also, the parameters are unknown but random quantities and described distributionally.

Summary of the estimators

In frequentist approach, point estimates and their standard errors, along with the 95% confidence interval, describe not only the characteristics of the estimator but also the interval that covers the true parameter value 95% of times, on an average. In the Bayesian analysis, the inference about the parameter is usually obtained through their posterior means and quantiles. The highest probability density interval indicates the region of highest posterior probability of occurrence of the parameter. The Bayesian credible interval provides a probabilistic bounded region for the parameter value. However, the frequentist confidence interval describes a probability about the bounds given fixed parameter value.

Example 6

To estimate the incidence of CAD in people without any heart disease at baseline but having snorers with obstructive sleep apnea and snorers without obstructive sleep apnea, a study was conducted[17] on a total of 308 (245 males and 63 females) individuals reported in middle-aged males during a follow-up period of 7 years. Let us consider that the occurrence of any CAD among those having (a) snorers with obstructive sleep apnea and (b) snorers without obstructive sleep apnea, is a Binomial random variable, and the incidence rate has assumed to follow beta (a, b), for the present study: a = 4 and b = 22. The impact on change in the posterior distribution (beta-binomial) for the proportion of people who suffered from CAD due to different prior choices for the given data is presented in [Table 4] and presented in [Figure 1].
Table 4: Summary statistics of prior, likelihood, and posterior distributions

Click here to view
Figure 1: Density pattern of binomial likelihood, beta prior, and beta-binomial posterior distribution. (a) Snores with obstructive sleep apnea, (b) snores without obstructive sleep apnea.

Click here to view


Quality checking

In the frequentist paradigm, the comparison between groups was generally carried out by defining the errors of Type I (level of known significance) and Type II, the size of the effect, and/or the power of the test procedure. The final inference is based on the P value.[18] In the case of Bayesian method,[19] the analytical procedure allows the formal incorporation of prior information. Here, the comparison between the groups is generally done by comparing the posterior predictive distributions,[20],[21] the posterior sensitivity is based on the prior forms, and the final inferences were made using the Bayes' factors.[6],[22],[23] The Bayes' factor (BF01) is the ratio of the posterior odds over the ratio of the prior odds. The rule of thumb[23] to infer is as follows, if log10(BF01) varies between 0 and 0.5, the evidence against null hypothesis H0 will be poor, if log10(BF01) lies between 0.5 and 1, it is substantial, if it is between 1 and 2, it is strong, and if it is above 2, it is decisive.

To demonstrate the practical significance and applicability of the Bayesian method, we discussed through below given example of estrogen and progesterone receptors status of tumor assessed from 20 breast cancer patients.

Example 7

Suppose the estrogen and progesterone receptors status of the tumor are assessed from 20 patients with locally advanced breast cancer patients. Here, we are trying to test whether the estrogen and progesterone receptors status of tumor are independent [Table 5].
Table 5: Cross-tabulation of status of estrogen and progesterone receptors among breast cancer patients

Click here to view


As the P = 0.05402, based on the Chi-square test, is greater than 0.05 significance level, we do not reject the null hypothesis that the status of progesterone and estrogen receptors is independent among breast cancer patients.

Let E be the event that the tumor is estrogen receptor-positive and F be the event that it is progesterone receptor-positive. Then,







The conditional probability that an individual progesterone receptor, given the tumor is estrogen receptor-positive, is



However, the conditional probability that an individual progesterone receptor, given the tumor is estrogen receptor-negative, is



The ratio of posterior odds and the prior odds in favor of event estrogen receptor, E, is , which is known as the Bayes factor in favor of event E, is





The value of logarithm of Bayes factor (1.68) suggests strong evidence of impact of estrogen receptor on the progesterone receptor among patients having breast cancer.







In this introductory article, an attempt is being made to describe the functionality of the Bayesian technique that can be used for analyzing a wide variety of data for which prior information is available and that its incorporation minimizing uncertainty in making effective clinical decisions. Here, we described how the Bayes' theorem updates the priorly available belief and information in the form of posterior probability density function conditioning on the available observations. Such is the efficacy and strength of Bayesian techniques, which produce simple rules and a sophisticated analytical procedure for analysis of medical data. In addition, Bayesian techniques provided rules for scientific reasoning for testing the underlying hypothesis about the parameter in relation to the observed data and quantified the belief in terms of probability.

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank the anonymous referees and Prof. Dr. Sandeep Seth, Department of Cardiology, All India Institute of Medical Sciences, for their constructive comments and suggestions to improve the quality of this manuscript.

Author's contributions

All authors have made equal and substantial contributions to the work reported in the manuscript.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
  References Top

1.
Clyde M, George EI. Model uncertainty. Statistical science 2004;19:81-94.  Back to cited text no. 1
    
2.
Cornfield J. Bayes theorem. Review of the International Statistical Institute; 1967. p. 34-49.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Goodman SN. Toward evidence-based medical statistics. 1: The P value fallacy. Ann Intern Med 1999a;130:995-1004.  Back to cited text no. 3
    
4.
Berger JO, Sellke T. Testing a point null hypothesis: The irreconcilability of P values and evidence. J Am Stat Assoc 1987;82:112-22.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Robert CP. The Bayesian Choice. New York: Springer; 2007.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Goodman SN. Toward evidence-based medical statistics. 2: The Bayes factor. Ann Intern Med 1999b;130:1005-13.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Bernardinelli L, Clayton D, Montomoli C. Bayesian estimates of disease maps: How important are priors? Stat Med 1995;14:2411-31.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Holzer M, Müllner M, Sterz F, Robak O, Kliegel A, Losert H, et al. Efficacy and safety of endovascular cooling after cardiac arrest: Cohort study and Bayesian approach. Stroke 2006;37:1792-7.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Rifkin RD, Hood WB Jr. Bayesian analysis of electrocardiographic exercise stress testing. N Engl J Med 1977;297:681-6.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Wang Z, Salah MB, Gu B, Islam A, Goela A, Li S. Direct estimation of cardiac biventricular volumes with an adapted Bayesian formulation. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 2014;61:1251-60.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
L'Italien GJ, Paul SD, Hendel RC, Leppo JA, Cohen MC, Fleisher LA, et al. Development and validation of a Bayesian model for perioperative cardiac risk assessment in a cohort of 1,081 vascular surgical candidates. J Am Coll Cardiol 1996;27:779-86.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
Thiele H, Akin I, Sandri M, de Waha-Thiele S, Meyer-Saraei R, Fuernau G, et al. One-year outcomes after PCI strategies in cardiogenic shock. N Engl J Med 2018;379:1699-710.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Reardon MJ, Van Mieghem NM, Popma JJ, Kleiman NS, Søndergaard L, Mumtaz M, et al. Surgical or transcatheter aortic-valve replacement in intermediate-risk patients. N Engl J Med 2017;376:1321-31.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Staniloff HM, Diamond GA, Freeman MR, Berman DS, Forrester JS. Simplified application of Bayesian analysis to multiple cardiologic tests. Clin Cardiol 1982;5:630-6.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Bernardo JM, Smith AF. Bayesian Theory. Vol. 405. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2009.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Carlin BP, Louis TA. Bayes and Empirical Bayes Methods for Data Analysis. 2nd edn. Hertfordshire, U.K.: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2000.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Peker Y, Carlson J, Hedner J. Increased incidence of coronary artery disease in sleep apnoea: A long-term follow-up. Eur Respir J 2006;28:596-602.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Laber EB, Shedden K. Statistical significance and the dichotomization of evidence: The relevance of the lASA statement on statistical significance and P values for statisticians. J Am Stat Assoc 2017;112:902-4.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Gelman A, Meng XL, Stern H. Posterior predictive assessment of model fitness via realized discrepancies. Stat Sin 1996;6:733-60.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Geisser S, Eddy WF. A predictive approach to model selection. J Am Stat Assoc 1979;74:153-60.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Agresti A, Min Y. Frequentist performance of Bayesian confidence intervals for comparing proportions in 2 × 2 contingency tables. Biometrics 2005;61:515-23.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Ashby D, Hutton J, McGee M. Simple Bayesian analyses for case-control studies in cancer epidemiology. Statistician 1993;42:385-97.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes factors. J Am StatAssoc 1995;90:773.  Back to cited text no. 23
    


    Figures

  [Figure 1]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4], [Table 5]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
Access Statistics
Email Alert *
Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)

 
  In this article
   Abstract
  Introduction
  Example 4
   Fundamentals Of ...
   Methodological D...
  Conclusion
   References
   Article Figures
   Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed402    
    Printed39    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded120    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal


[TAG2]
[TAG3]
[TAG4]